Lost? Call a Cow...

Discussion in 'General Discussion' started by Seacowboys, May 3, 2015.


  1. Seacowboys

    Seacowboys Senior Member Founding Member

    Magnetic alignment in grazing and resting cattle and deer
    Magnetic alignment in grazing and resting cattle and deer
    1. Sabine Begall*,,
    2. Jaroslav Červený,§,
    3. Julia Neef*,
    4. Oldřich Vojtěch,, and
    5. Hynek Burda*
    Author Affiliations

    1. Edited by Simon A. Levin, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, and approved July 17, 2008 (received for review April 15, 2008)
    1. Abstract
    2. Full Text
    3. Authors & Info
    4. Figures
    5. Metrics
    6. Related Content


    Next Section
    Abstract
    We demonstrate by means of simple, noninvasive methods (analysis of satellite images, field observations, and measuring “deer beds” in snow) that domestic cattle (n = 8,510 in 308 pastures) across the globe, and grazing and resting red and roe deer (n = 2,974 at 241 localities), align their body axes in roughly a north–south direction. Direct observations of roe deer revealed that animals orient their heads northward when grazing or resting. Amazingly, this ubiquitous phenomenon does not seem to have been noticed by herdsmen, ranchers, or hunters. Because wind and light conditions could be excluded as a common denominator determining the body axis orientation, magnetic alignment is the most parsimonious explanation. To test the hypothesis that cattle orient their body axes along the field lines of the Earth's magnetic field, we analyzed the body orientation of cattle from localities with high magnetic declination. Here, magnetic north was a better predictor than geographic north. This study reveals the magnetic alignment in large mammals based on statistically sufficient sample sizes. Our findings open horizons for the study of magnetoreception in general and are of potential significance for applied ethology (husbandry, animal welfare). They challenge neuroscientists and biophysics to explain the proximate mechanisms.

    Farmers and attentive nature and countryside observers know that most cattle and sheep, when grazing, face the same way. Many of them ask for the reason and which factors determine the direction in which they align. The farmers' wisdom and experience indicate that cattle face into the wind, whereas sheep face away from the wind; the animals expose the maximum body surface area to the sun when sun basking in cold but sunny times of the day. Several scientific studies also addressed alignment of grazing cattle and sheep from the point of behavioral thermoregulation, i.e., they focused on alignment under suboptimal weather conditions. Thus it was confirmed that cattle stand perpendicular to the sun on cold, sunny days, especially in the early morning, maximizing the surface area exposed to short-wave radiation and gaining heat. On the other hand, cattle orient parallel with strong winds during winter, which minimizes the area exposed to convective heat loss associated with wind (e.g., ref. 1 and references therein). However, to the best of our knowledge, the farmers' wisdom and scientific studies have not provided answers (and even do not address the question) about which factors determine common alignment of cattle (and sheep) within one herd under favorable, nonstressful conditions (windless, sunless days, with optimal or near-optimal temperature). Furthermore, apparently there is no information whether the cattle show any common alignment during night grazing periods and when resting. Also to the best of our knowledge, no scientific study (or common hunters' wisdom) addresses whether wild ruminants (like deer) also predictably align when grazing or resting.

    In this study, we address these questions by combining several methodical approaches. First, we recorded body alignment of cattle in satellite images provided by Google Earth. In this manner we received scan-sampling data on alignment of animals in diverse localities across the globe and in diverse times, making it unlikely that effective direction of each of the factors (wind, sun, and temperature) was a common key factor of the alignment in all places and times. Second, we observed alignment in grazing and resting roe deer at different times of the day (even at night) in diverse localities, under diverse climatic conditions. Third, we analyzed the alignment of “beds” (body prints in snow of resting animals) of red deer and roe deer. We demonstrate that in all cases the animals tend to show a roughly north–south (N-S) body alignment, and we argue that a further extrinsic cue, the magnetic field of the Earth, has to be considered as a factor affecting spatial orientation in cattle and deer.

    Magnetoreception is a widespread, although enigmatic, sensory ability. Behavioral experiments have demonstrated that diverse animals, including representatives of six vertebrate classes, can use the magnetic field of the Earth as a cue for spatial orientation (2). Among mammals, robust evidence for magnetic compass orientation has been obtained only recently for, thus far, just a few rodent species (37) and one bat species (8). Magnetic compass orientation has been suggested also for humans and some larger mammals, such as horses and cetaceans. Its evidence is, however, questionable and mainly only anecdotal (2). Surely, the investigation of magnetic orientation in large mammals under reproducible controlled laboratory conditions involving sufficiently large sample sizes is difficult, if not impossible. While most experiments on mammalian magnetic orientation have been based on the study of homing or learning achievements, spontaneous (innate) magnetic behavioral responses (and their subsequent manipulation) have remained largely unstudied and untapped (but see the study of magnetoreception in mole-rats in refs.3, 4, and 911).

    Previous SectionNext Section
    Results
    Body Position of Cattle.

    Body axes of cattle (Bos primigenius) of 308 evaluated herds/pastures (displayed on satellite images in Google Earth) showed a significant deviation from random distribution (Rayleigh test, P < 0.00001) with a preference for a rough N-S direction (mean vector: 5.4°/185.4° with geographic north as reference). Because declination was small for most pastures chosen (i.e., magnetic north being close to geographic north), cattle were also roughly N-S oriented with respect to magnetic north (mean vector: 6.4°/186.4°, P < 0.00001). Taken the pastures separately, cattle show significant axial body orientation with a mean vector of 1.2°/181.2° in Europe, 3.7°/183.7° in Asia, 12.1°/192.1° in Australia, 30.9°/210.9° in Africa, 32.0°/212.0° in South America, and 347.5°/167.5° in North America (Table 1 and Fig. 1A; all data with reference to magnetic north). To evaluate whether geographic or magnetic north better predicts cattle orientation, we separately evaluated pastures originating from localities with naturally high positive and negative declination values. The mean vectors calculated for localities with high positive and negative declination differed significantly when geographic north was used as a reference (U2 = 0.39; P < 0.001). When using magnetic north as a reference the difference between the mean vectors was not significant (U2 = 0.15; P > 0.1), indicating that magnetic north is a much better predictor for the observed alignment to N-S than geographic north (Table 2).

    Table 1.
    Basic circular statistics for axial directions of cows from six continents


    [​IMG]
    Fig. 1.
    Axial data revealing the N-S alignment in three ruminant species under study. (A) Cattle. (B) Roe deer. (C) Red deer. Each pair of dots (located on opposite sites within the unit circle) represents the direction of the axial mean vector of the animals' body position at one locality. The mean vector calculated over all localities of the respective species is indicated by the double-headed arrow. The length of the arrow represents the r-value (length of the mean vector), dotted circles indicate the 0.01-level of significance. Triangles positioned outside the unit circle indicate the mean vectors of the cattle data subdivided into the six continents (dotted: North America; gray: Asia; checkered: Europe; striped: Australia; black: Africa; white: South America) (A) and the mean vectors of resting (black) and grazing (white) deer, and of deer beds (dotted) (B: roe deer; C: red deer).


    Table 2.
    Geographic north versus magnetic north as references for body orientation of cattle originating from localities with naturally high declination values


    Cattle's body orientation was independent from sun position because no significant correlation between axial shadow direction (as an indicator for sun position) and the cattle's body axes have been found (circular correlation; r = 0.159; n = 103 herds).

    Body Position of Deer.

    Body axes of grazing roe deer (Capreolus capreolus) deviate significantly from random distribution (Rayleigh test, P < 0.00001) revealing alignment to approximately N-S direction (mean vector: 9.0°/189.0°, r = 0.81, n = 152 herds); when resting, they align their body axis along 10.7°/190.7°, r = 0.93, (n = 28 herds). Also body axes of grazing European red deer (Cervus elaphus) showed a significant deviation from random distribution (Rayleigh test, P < 0.00001) and were directed in a roughly N-S direction (9.7°/189.7°, r = 0.86; n = 16 herds). Deer beds in the snow, i.e., body prints of resting or sleeping animals, were in the same range (roe deer: 7.6°/187.6°, r = 0.86°, n = 21 herds; red deer: 10.5°/190.5°, r = 0.9, n = 24 herds) (Table 3 and Fig. 1 B and C).

    Table 3.
    Basic circular statistics for axial directions of deer from the Czech Republic


    Direct observations revealed that the majority of grazing and resting deer face northward (angular data for roe deer: 13.2°, r = 0.62, n = 166 herds; angular data for red deer: 34.3°, r = 0.47, n = 16 herds). When looking around and checking the surroundings, the animals just turn their heads but do not change the direction of their body axis. They change their body direction for short periods when moving to the next grazing place.

    There was no significant correlation between the body position of roe deer and the time of day representing the position of the sun (circular correlation: r = 0.002).

    Previous SectionNext Section
    Discussion
    Although the detailed dates of the satellite images are not provided by Google Earth, most views were apparently made on cloudless sunny days, judging from short shades, mainly around midday. Based on the vegetation, all images from Europe and the United States originated from summer times, and those from the southern continents covered both dry and rainy seasons. Given the number and variety of localities distributed geographically, ecologically, and over time, the apparently calm weather situation, and the high position of the sun, the direction of the wind and the position of the sun are unlikely to be of major influence on the cattle's body orientation in our sample. They play, if at all, only a minor role and may be the “exceptions to the rule” (i.e., cases, when the animals were not aligned in a N-S direction).

    Wind.

    If the wind was the primary factor determining alignment of the evaluated cattle, it has to be presumed that there were windy (and not windless) conditions in most of the 308 sampled pastures distributed geographically and in time. We had to presume furthermore that winds were strong and blowing mainly from northern or southern directions. This is highly improbable, because in the Northern Hemisphere westerlies and in the Southern Hemisphere “south eastern trades” are prevailing, and generally, airflow is deflected by the Coriolis force and tends to follow more east–west (E-W) direction rather than N-S (Encyclopedia of the Atmospheric Environment, www.ace.mmu.ac.uk/eae/Climate/Older/Wind_Belts.html). Concordantly, wind atlases show that prevailing winds in the respective countries are variable throughout the year and mostly westerly. Regionally and locally, weaker winds are, however, highly variable throughout the year. Taken together, should wind be the determining factor of alignment, either random distribution of body orientation would be expected (if winds were weak or negligible, as probably was the situation in most cases), or the cattle would be oriented in west–east or northwest–southeast (if winds were strong and cattle were facing into the wind).

    The wind factor can be excluded also for alignment of resting deer, because deer search for wind-protected places deep in the forests to rest (and even if it is windy, the wind in the forest is dampened and changes its direction locally and unpredictably). Most beds were fresh, originating from the previous night. Climatic data for those particular nights indicate windless situations or winds blowing from different directions. Because the direct observation of grazing and resting deer took place mainly during windless days (or during days when the wind was negligible and in any case blowing from different directions at different recording times and localities), the influence of wind can be excluded as well.

    Sun.

    Theoretically, the sun may influence body alignment in three different ways: thermoregulation, sun compass, and looking away to avoid dazzling. Sun basking is displayed by animals on cold, sunny days, mostly in the morning (when shades are long), after cold nights. Animals are standing so that they do not shade each other, and mostly are not grazing. No images that would suggest sun basking were found. Cattle facing heat stress do not graze, and they look for cover in tree shade, etc., if available. There were no images in our sample that would raise suspicion that the cattle were heat-stressed. We observed sun basking in roe deer; yet these records were not included into the analysis. In all observations of grazing or resting red and roe deer that were included into the analyses behavioral thermoregulation could be clearly excluded. Thermoregulation as an alignment-determining factor can be excluded in night observations and alignment of night deer beds.

    The sun compass is known to play an important role in navigation (but not in alignment) of e.g., insects and migratory songbirds. It is known that these animals can perceive polarized light so that they can orient by means of a sun compass even on cloudy days (cf. refs. 12 and 13 and references therein). The animals known to perceive polarized light and use the sun compass (primarily for long-distance navigation) are basically diurnal, with vision being a dominant sense. On the contrary, ruminants are active throughout the day with several nocturnal grazing periods, also in deep winter. Moreover, although clear behavioral evidence is still missing, the retinal parameters speak clearly against the capacity of polarized light perception (L. Peichl and H. Wässle, personal communication). Facing away from the sun, to avoid dazzling, could play a role in the morning or late afternoon, when the sun is low, and would result in E-W orientation of animals. This aspect can be clearly excluded in most of the satellite images (short shades), all of the direct observations (most of them being done on cloudy days), night observations, and deer beds.

    Furthermore, there was no correlation between the position of roe deer and the time of day when the observation took place, meaning that the position of the sun had no influence on deer orientation.

    Magnetic Field.

    Because climatic factors like wind, sun, or temperature were apparently not common directional key factors explaining ubiquitous alignment, we conclude that the magnetic field is the only common and most likely factor responsible for the observed alignment. Our analysis of cattle at localities with naturally high positive and negative declinations (compare Table 2) clearly provides the crucial proof in favor of the Earth's magnetic field being the responsive cue.

    Magnetic Alignment.

    Magnetic alignment is a spontaneous behavioral expression of magnetoreception that appears particularly in resting animals when body orientation is not controlled by other factors (2). Earlier laboratory studies confirmed a certain preference for alignment to the magnetic field lines for several insect groups like flies, termites, and honey bees (1417); further studies are reviewed in ref. 2. Among vertebrates, fishes (namely goldfish and eel) represent the only group for which alignment behaviors have been reported (2, 18). Recently, it was reported that pigeons show a tendency to align their flight in directions relative to the intensity of the geomagnetic field (19). In contrast to the easy recording of body alignment, its statistical evaluation is of a less trivial character. Alignment data are bimodal or quadrimodal, and usually <2/3 of the observed individuals express it (2). Magnetic alignment per se does not require magnetoperception, i.e., conscious sensing of the geomagnetic field, and it does not necessarily imply the use of a magnetic compass for spatial long-distance orientation and navigation. Nevertheless, it surely requires some kind of magnetoreception. Observations made on grazing roe deer, and evaluation of fresh deer beds, where head and rear ends of the bed are easily recognizable, suggest that the recorded phenomenon represents not just a simple bimodal magnetic alignment of the body axis but even head orientation in the northern direction. Similarly, the angular analysis of data for grazing red deer revealed that the majority of animals orient their heads northward. However, within groups of animals, approximately one-third of the deer orient their heads southward, resulting in a grand mean vector of 34° (angular analysis). This differential alignment may represent an antipredatory behavior, as in the region of recordings, the lynx occurs.

    The biological significance of the shown magnetic alignment remains enigmatic. It has been speculated that maintaining a symmetric position to the field lines somehow influences certain physiological processes (ref.2 and references therein). Indeed, in humans the rapid eye movement latency is shortened in the E-W position of sleepers compared with the N-S position (20), and statistically significant differences in the EEG of normal subjects have been found, depending on whether the subjects sit facing the N-S or E-W direction (21). Maintaining a certain magnetic direction may provide also a constant directional reference for spatial orientation, which might be useful e.g., after disturbance and fast escape. Noteworthy, all of the studied ruminants are social animals, with large home ranges, moving over large distances, originally in habitats (dense forests or grassland) without apparent landmarks. Our results call for an in-depth study of this phenomenon and challenge neuroscientists, biochemists, and physicists to study the proximate mechanisms and biological significance of magnetic alignment. It is amazing that this ubiquitous conspicuous phenomenon apparently has remained unnoticed by herdsmen and hunters for thousands of years.

    Previous SectionNext Section
    Materials and Methods
    Analysis of Body Position of Cattle Using Google Earth.

    We determined the axial directions of 8,510 cattle of 308 randomly selected localities (pastures) from six continents: Africa (Morocco, South Africa), Asia (India), Australia, Europe (Belgium, Denmark, France, Germany, Ireland, Netherlands, Russia, United Kingdom), North America (Connecticut, Kansas, Massachusetts, Montana, New York, Oregon, Texas), and South America (Argentina) by using satellite images freely available at Google Earth mapping services. Care was taken to evaluate only pastures in the flat country. The recordings include both sexes and diverse races of both dairy and beef cattle. The resolution of most satellite images in Google Earth did not allow clear and fast distinction between the individuals' head and rear, so records were confined to the body axis. Also, we did not distinguish between grazing, resting, and moving individuals. Chosen eye altitude depended on resolution and ranged between 45 and 1,730 m. Images of bad resolution and pastures located near the sea, at the hillside, or near human settlements were not selected. Screenshots of the chosen pastures were copied from Google Earth and pasted into Microsoft Powerpoint. Cattle moving on trails or standing at feeding troughs or watering places and calves being close to (suckler) cows were excluded from further analysis. We marked the cattle's longitudinal axis by drawing a straight line with the Powerpoint drawing tools and estimated for each animal separately its direction to the nearest 5° by overlaying a circular scale with 10° steps. Because we could not always distinguish the animals' front and rear, bidirectional analysis was the method of choice [i.e., data are doubled (modulo 360) before being analyzed, and the resulting mean vector is then back-converted, thus ranging in the interval (0°; 180°)]. Cattle of the same herd might not orient independently of each other, and we therefore calculated a single mean vector per pasture that was used in further analysis (Rayleigh test). All axial values are reported as XX°/XX° (N–S).

    To estimate whether geographic north or magnetic north better predicts the body alignment, we investigated pastures at localities with high declination (Connecticut: −14.8°; Massachusetts: −14.7°; New York: −14°; Australia: +8°; +12.3°; Montana: +10°; +14.8°; Oregon: +17.5°). Watson's U2 test was used to test for significant differences between the two mean vectors representing alignment in cattle from localities with high negative and high positive declination. This test was conducted for geographic north and magnetic north separately.

    Satellite images provided by Google Earth are oriented with respect to geographic north, and to correct the mean vectors for declination we used the following formula to obtain mean vectors (with respect to magnetic north): mean vectormagneticN = (mean vectorgeogrN − mean declination) modulo 180. We used the online calculator of the National Geophysical Data Center (www.ngdc.noaa.gov/seg/geomag/jsp/IGRFWMM.jsp) to calculate magnetic parameters for each locality separately for the period 2000–2007 (step size: 1 year) and averaged the declination for the respective 8 years. The corrected mean vectors were then tested for uniformity by using the Rayleigh test with Z = nr2 (significance level set to α = 0.01). As repetitions of the “experiments” with the same herd of cattle are impossible, only first-order statistics could be performed. Calculations were performed (i) for all localities together and (ii) for each continent separately.

    Sun Position and Cattle Orientation.

    To evaluate a possible influence of the sun position and the cattle's body position, we performed a circular correlation for these parameters. We determined the sun position indirectly by evaluating the shadow direction. Only those images have been chosen where the shadow direction could be clearly identified (n = 103 localities).

    Analysis of Body Position of Deer (Field Observation).

    Body position of 2,974 deer (in 227 localities) was recorded in the Czech Republic. Axial directions of deer were based on measuring beds of animals that had rested in the snow (roe deer: n = 430 in 21 localities; red deer: n = 917 in 24 localities) and on direct snapshot observations of grazing and resting deer (roe deer: n = 1,080 in 152 localities, red deer:n = 145 in 16 localities). Recorded red deer beds were distributed in deep forests of the Sumava Mountains National Park that represent undisturbed localities chosen by deer for overwintering for generations. Grazing roe deer were observed at different times in the winter of 2007/2008 in a variety of habitats, in localities encompassing both the national park area and the agricultural landscape in the center of the Czech Republic. Animals that were obviously sun-basking were not taken into account. The animals did not notice the observer, or, being habituated, did not apparently react to him. Only records of resting or grazing (i.e., undisturbed) animals were analyzed. Standing and moving animals were not considered. Climatic data (wind, sun, temperature) had been recorded on the day the observation of grazing deer took place or the day before we measured the deer beds.

    All directions were measured ± 5° with a compass. Only for the directly observed grazing and resting deer we distinguished between front and rear (angular data). All other data are bidirectional. However, to compare data for resting and grazing roe deer with those for beds, we classified all data as axial. As with the cattle, only one mean vector per deer locality was taken into account to obtain statistical independence. Again, the Rayleigh test was applied to test for significant deviations from uniform distribution of the mean vectors.

    All circular statistics were calculated with Oriana 2.0 (Kovach Computing).

    Sun Position and Roe Deer Orientation.

    The position of the sun could be deviated by the exact time of the day. Circular correlation has been tested for the parameters time of day and roe deer position.

    Previous SectionNext Section
    Acknowledgments
    We thank Marek Drha for assistance in the collection of field data and John Philips, Regina E. Moritz, and one anonymous reviewer for providing valuable comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.

    Previous SectionNext Section
    Footnotes
    • †To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: sabine.begall@uni-due.de
    • Author contributions: J.Č. and H.B. designed research; S.B., J.Č., O.V., and H.B. performed research; S.B. and J.N. analyzed data; and S.B. and H.B. wrote the paper.

    • The authors declare no conflict of interest.

    • This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

    • © 2008 by The National Academy of Sciences of the USA
    Previous Section

    References
      1. Olson BE,
      2. Wallander RT
      (2001) Orientation of beef cattle grazing foothill winter range in Montana.Proc W Sect Amer Soc Animal Sci 52:1–6.
      Google Scholar
      1. Wiltschko R,
      2. Wiltschko W
      (1995) Magnetic Orientation in Animals (Springer, Berlin).
      Google Scholar
      1. Burda H,
      2. Marhold S,
      3. Westenberger T,
      4. Wiltschko W,
      5. Wiltschko R
      (1990) Magnetic compass orientation in the subterranean rodent Cryptomys hottentotus (Bathyergidae, Rodentia) Experientia46:528–530.
      CrossRefMedlineWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar
      1. Burda H,
      2. et al.
      (1991) Magnetic orientation in subterranean mole rats of the superspecies Spalax ehrenbergi: Experiments, patterns, and memory. Isr J Zool 37:182–183.
      Google Scholar
      1. Deutschlander ME,
      2. et al.
      (2003) Learned magnetic compass orientation by the Siberian hamster,Phodopus sungorus. Anim Behav 65:779–786.
      CrossRefWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar
      1. Kimchi T,
      2. Terkel J
      (2001) Magnetic compass orientation in the blind mole rat Spalax ehrenbergi. J Exp Biol 204:751–758.
      Abstract
      1. Muheim R,
      2. Edgar NM,
      3. Sloan KA,
      4. Phillips JB
      (2006) Magnetic compass orientation in C57BL/6 mice. Learn Behav 34:366–373.
      MedlineWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar
      1. Holland RA,
      2. Thorup K,
      3. Vonhof M,
      4. Cochran WW,
      5. Wikelski M
      (2006) Bat orientation using Earth's magnetic field. Nature 444:653–702.
      Google Scholar
      1. Marhold S,
      2. Wiltschko W,
      3. Burda H
      (1997) A magnetic polarity compass for direction finding in a subterranean mammal. Naturwissenschaften 84:421–423.
      CrossRefWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar
      1. Němec P,
      2. Altmann J,
      3. Marhold S,
      4. Burda H,
      5. Oelschläger HAH
      (2001) Neuroanatomy of magnetoreception: The superior colliculus involved in magnetic orientation in a mammal. Science294:366–368.
      Abstract/FREE Full Text
      1. Wegner RE,
      2. Begall S,
      3. Burda H
      (2006) Magnetic compass in the cornea: Local anaesthesia impairs orientation in a mammal. J Exp Biol 209:4747–4750.
      Abstract/FREE Full Text
      1. Wehner R
      (1976) Polarized-light navigation by insects. Sci Am 23:106–115.
      Google Scholar
      1. Muheim R,
      2. Phillips JB,
      3. Akesson S
      (2006) Polarized light cues underlie compass calibration in migratory songbirds. Science 313:837–839.
      Abstract/FREE Full Text
      1. Becker G,
      2. Speck U
      (1964) Examinations on magnetic field orientation in Dipterans. Z Vergl Physiol49:301–340.
      CrossRefGoogle Scholar
      1. Becker G
      (1963) Resting position according to magnetic direction, magnetic orientation in termites.Naturwissenschaften 50:455.
      Google Scholar
      1. Altman G
      (1981) Investigation of magnetotaxis of the honey bee, Apis melifica L. Anz Schädlingskd Pflanzenschutz Umweltschutz 54:177–179.
      CrossRefGoogle Scholar
      1. Becker G
      (1964) Reactions of insects on magnetic fields, electric fields, and the atmosphere. Z Angew Entomol 54:75–88.
      Google Scholar
      1. Becker G
      (1974) Influence of the Earth's magnetic field on the directional behavior of gold fish.Naturwissenschaften 61:220–221.
      MedlineWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar
      1. Dennis TE,
      2. Rayner MJ,
      3. Walker MM
      (2007) Evidence that pigeons orient to geomagnetic intensity during homing. Proc R Soc London Ser B 274:1153–1158.
      Abstract/FREE Full Text
      1. Ruhenstroth-Bauer G,
      2. Rüther E,
      3. Reinertshofer TH
      (1987) Dependence of a sleeping parameter from the N-S or E-W sleeping direction. Zeitschr Naturf C 42:1140–1142.
      Google Scholar
      1. Ruhenstroth-Bauer G,
      2. et al.
      (1993) Influence of the Earth's magnetic field on resting and activated EEG mapping in normal subjects. Int J Neurosci 73:195–201.
      CrossRefMedlineWeb of ScienceGoogle Scholar
     
  2. Gray Wolf

    Gray Wolf Monkey+++

    I wonder how many millions of our taxpayer dollars were spent on that fairly useless bit of trivia?
     
  3. kellory

    kellory An unemployed Jester, is nobody's fool. Banned

    I can't see much use to any of it, even if true. A deer's nose is seven times better than a blood hound's nose, and THAT is it's primary defense.so no matter what direction it is facing, it is scent that will trigger the alert.
    Their eyesight is different than ours, and covers everything but roughly 60° behind their heads, and great at light gathering, but of poor vision, and unable to see reds. (They do not have the correct rods and cones to see that color wavelength).
    So it is the wind, not the direction they face, that matters most to their survival.
    In fact, a mature buck does not need to even approach a group of does to determine if one or more are ready to breed, but travel downwind of the group, at right angles to the wind, and simply sample the breeze in passing.
     
  4. ghrit

    ghrit Bad company Administrator Founding Member

    Interesting concept, but I'm not so sure that there will always be time to take a statistical sampling of the herd.
     
    Tully Mars, Ganado and DarkLight like this.
  5. BTPost

    BTPost Stumpy Old Fart,Deadman Walking, Snow Monkey Moderator

    A compass works a LOT Better.... or even Magnetize the Sewing Needle in your BOB, and hang it from a piece of Thread... You should have both in there, ANYWAY..... I mean REALLY, DUH.....
     
    sec_monkey and DarkLight like this.
  6. DarkLight

    DarkLight Live Long and Prosper - On Hiatus

    Especially in the absense of a herd of cattle or deer.

    And how exactly would you shoot an azimuth using a cow?
     
  7. Seacowboys

    Seacowboys Senior Member Founding Member

    You don't always have a compass but cows are hiding everywhere...
     
    Tully Mars, Ganado and chelloveck like this.
  8. Witch Doctor 01

    Witch Doctor 01 Mojo Maker

    with my luck any cow I found would be an outlier...
     
    Tully Mars, Ganado and chelloveck like this.
  9. chelloveck

    chelloveck Diabolus Causidicus

    Some here may instead answer the question by trying to shoot a cow using an azimuth. That sometimes used to happen to kangaroos at the field firing range at Singleton with mortars.

     
    Ganado likes this.
  10. UncleMorgan

    UncleMorgan I like peeling bananas and (occasionally) people.

    Very cool post!
    Now, if a person is tromping around in the bush, LOHA, simply observing the wildlife--or the alignment of a bedsite--may provide a clue to direction.
    Does it beat a magnetic compass? No. GPS? No. Analog watch and sun position? No. Two pegs in the dirt and some shadows? No. But it could work, and there may come a time when a person needs to use that exact method of finding North.
    Good-o, mate: That's one more tool in the survival toolbox.

    And, now you might want to maybe work that wind from the North when you're on the ground, stalking deer. Or cows.
     
  11. kellory

    kellory An unemployed Jester, is nobody's fool. Banned

    "And, now you might want to maybe work that wind from the North when you're on the ground, stalking deer. Or cows."

    Now THAT would be beyond useless, unless you were part of a deer or cattle drive. The idea of hunting the wind, is to approach or have the game approach, so they can NOT smell you.
    Unless I misunderstand your meaning, that method would guarantee failure.
     
  12. Seacowboys

    Seacowboys Senior Member Founding Member

     
    oldawg and Tully Mars like this.
  13. kellory

    kellory An unemployed Jester, is nobody's fool. Banned

    upload_2015-5-4_7-54-52.
    DAMN , I wish I remembered my compass!!!
     
    Motomom34, oldawg, Ganado and 2 others like this.
  14. Tully Mars

    Tully Mars Metal weldin' monkey

    Every time the wife sees cows laying down she says it's a sign of rain. I just tell her they are lazy southern cows;)
     
    Motomom34 likes this.
  15. madmax

    madmax Far right. Bipolar. Veteran. Don't push me.

    This is me making fart noises at that research.

    ...and I am AGAIN pissed at the useless money put into BS like this.

    BTW. Farm raised. Dad was a veterinarian. I smelled like horse and cow poo until 15. Rescue was every thing on the farm. We had a menagerie.
     
    Ganado and Tully Mars like this.
  16. Motomom34

    Motomom34 Monkey+++

    My first thought was this reminds me of Feng Shui. I have heard that you should sleep with your feet facing West, someone studied that but probably not as in depth as this cow article. I wonder how a polar shift would affect them.
     
survivalmonkey SSL seal        survivalmonkey.com warrant canary
17282WuJHksJ9798f34razfKbPATqTq9E7